The Impact of Graphic Injury Photographs

The Impact of Graphic Injury Photographs on Liability Verdicts and Non-Economic Damage Awards

​​​​​​​

1. Introduction


With the ever increasing availability of photographic evidence and the trend toward the admissibility of graphic visual images, it has become even more important to understand how these pictures influence jurors' verdicts. Little research has been conducted to address this important issue. What research has been completed suggests that graphic photographic evidence can have an improper effect in the criminal arena by influencing conviction rates (See Douglas, Lynn, & Ogloff, 1997; Bright & Goodman-Delahunty, 2006). The literature on the impact of vivid injury photographs in the civil setting suggests that there is a relationship between this evidence and research participants' damage awards (See Oliver & Griffitt, 1976, Whalen & Blanchard 1982).

In civil litigation, photographic images depicting the severity of an injury are submitted during the trial to purportedly help the jury assess economic (e.g., lost wages) and non-economic damages. Photos of a burned hand taken immediately after an accident may help a jury assess the pain and suffering associated with the injury. The application of this evidence is considered proper when used in this limited capacity. However, before a jury weighs damages, it must first find that the defendant was negligent and that this negligence caused injury to the plaintiff. Arguing jurors' improper use of injury photographs to determine liability may mean these images should be excluded under FRE 403. According to 403, relevant evidence is inadmissible if it is found to be unfairly prejudicial (emphasis added):

Although relevant, evidence may be excluded if its probative value is substantially outweighed by the danger of unfair prejudice, confusion of the issues, or misleading the jury, or by considerations of undue delay, waste of time, or needless presentation of cumulative evidence.

The current study examines the proper and improper use of vivid injury photographs in a civil dispute where the evidence favors a defense verdict. We assessed the impact of injury photographs on research participants' liability, causation, and non-economic damages verdicts (i.e., pain and suffering awards). The study also answers a question of strategic value: Can a defendant mitigate the influence that injury images have on liability verdicts and damage awards by providing counter photographs depicting improvement in the plaintiff's condition?

2. A Study of Impacts in a Product Liability Lawsuit


The research combined three conditions including identical written trial stories embedded with neutral photographs. The fact pattern was developed from a lawsuit that was resolved during trial. The original litigation stemmed from an injury to an 11-month-old infant who received severe, instant third-degree burns to his hands after he put them in the path of steam emitted from an operating vaporizer. The plaintiff (the infant) sued the manufacturer of the vaporizer claiming that: 1) the product was defective and 2) the warning labels were insufficient. The defense addressed both of the plaintiff's claims and asserted a comparative negligence argument against the parents. The final version of the trial story was slightly and intentionally skewed in the defendant's favor.

Injury photographs--two submitted by the plaintiff and two by the defense --were chosen from the original nine images provided by counsel. The plaintiff photographs depicted the burns to the infant's hands taken at the hospital within hours of the accident. In contrast, the defense pictures were taken over a year after the incident, following several successful skin grafts. These photographs illustrated marked improvement from the initial injury.

All three versions of the trial story were identical in content and contained the same neutral photographs of the parties, the vaporizer, and other relevant visual aids. The control condition detailed the injuries from both the plaintiff's and defendant's perspective, but did not offer any images of the injury. The second condition incorporated the two photos taken immediately after the accident into the plaintiff's case-in-chief. Finally, the third condition included the plaintiff photos but also incorporated the post-recovery images in the defendant's damages argument. After reading the online trial story, research participants were provided with the relevant jury instructions and then answered the verdict questions.

Research participants were randomly selected by a web-based survey company. The sample was drawn from a list of volunteers who opted in to complete web surveys for compensation in the form of points. The entire exercise was completed through written materials delivered through internet web access. The final sample comprised jury eligible participants from California, New York, Illinois, Texas, and Florida.

3. The Influence of Photographs On Juror Verdicts


As predicted, the photographs had a significant effect on final verdicts. The majority (58%) of participants in the first condition, who did not view any of the graphic photographs, found in favor of the defense. However, research participants who saw the plaintiff's injury photographs were significantly more likely to render plaintiff verdicts than participants who did not view the photograph, p = .03. Fifty-one percent (51%) of participants in this condition resulted in plaintiff verdicts. Yet, the defense was able to counter the illegitimate impact of the plaintiff's photographs on liability verdicts. Sixty-percent of participants who saw both the plaintiff and defense photos found for the defense.

The plaintiff's photographs also had an impact on damage awards. The median non-economic damages award for research participants who viewed the plaintiff photographs was significantly higher than that of those in the no photographs condition, p = .02. The defense photographs did not attenuate the impact that the plaintiff photos had on non-economic damage awards.

4. Practical Implications of Injury Photographs in Civil Litigation


The results from the present study indicate that injury photographs can and do have an improper effect on liability verdicts. Further, these photographs, which are ostensibly provided to assist in the assessment of damages, actually strengthen a somewhat weak plaintiff case. From a practical perspective, these findings have important implications. While injury photographs may be relevant for assessing damages, they also appear to spill over and contaminate questions on liability.

Research participants also improperly used photos proffered by the defense to determine liability. This finding suggests that the defendant may be able to mitigate the effects of graphic injury photos by offering photographs of its own. However, the improper use of counter photos does not justify or downplay the dangers posed by jurors using damages evidence to evaluate liability. Further, the post-surgery images submitted by the defense in this study illustrated a dramatic improvement in the plaintiff's condition. Such strong visual evidence is not always readily available to the defendant. Therefore, the defense may not be able to overcome the prejudicial impact that vivid injury photographs have on liability.

The results also suggest that plaintiff injury photographs have a significant, albeit proper, effect on non-economic damage awards. In contrast, defense photographs do not have an attenuating effect. This finding puts plaintiff counsel at a distinct advantage during the damages phase.

5. Legal Remedies


There are several approaches that could be employed by the Court to limit the prejudicial or improper use graphic photographs. Under the most aggressive scenario, the Court could exclude all graphic injury images on the basis of FRE 403. In light of the current trend toward admissibility, this approach is not likely to be employed any time soon.

Under a second scenario, the Court could provide a clear limiting instruction before photographs are submitted to the jury and an additional instruction before deliberations begin. Although this is the more widely accepted remedy, limiting instructions do not appear to be effective. There is a significant body of research within the cognitive and social psychological literature showing that participants have a difficult time suppressing thoughts when told to do so (See Wegner, Schneider, Carter, Ill & White, 1987).

The third approach calls for the bifurcation of the liability and damages phases, where the jury would only hear damage arguments after making a finding of liability against the defendant. Upon the jury returning a plaintiff verdict, both parties would then submit evidence in support of their competing damages claims. Although defense counsel may continue to have difficulty overcoming the impact that graphic injury photographs have on jurors damage awards, the improper use of such evidence to determine liability would be eliminated.

Bryan Edelman, Ph.D. is a trial consultant with the Jury Research Institute and is based in the Bay Area office in California. He is the author of Racial Prejudice, Juror Empathy, and Sentencing in Death Penalty Cases. While he has experience in the criminal arena, his primary focus is on civil litigation. You can read more about Dr. Edelman at [http://www.jri-inc.com].

6. References


Bright, D.A., & Goodman-Delahunty, J. (2006) Gruesome evidence and emotion. Anger, blame, and juror decision-making. Law and Human Behavior, 30, 183-202.

The Committee on the Judiciary House of Representatives. (2004). Federal Rules of Evidence. Washington, D.C.: U.S. Government Printing Office.

Douglas, K.S. Lynn, D.R. & Ogloff, J.R.P. (1997). The impact of graphic photographic evidence on mock juror decision in a murder trial: Probative or prejudicial. Law and Human Behavior, 21, 485-501.

Federal Rules of Evidence. The Committee on the Judiciary House of Representatives. Page 4. U.S. Government Printing Office: Washington (2004).

Oliver, E. & Griffitt, W. (1976). Emotional arousal and objective judgment. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 8, 399-400.

Wegner, D.M., Schneider, D.J., Carter, S.R., III, & White, T.L. (1987). Paradoxical effects of thought suppression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 5-13.

Whalen, D.H. & Blanchard, F.A. (1982). Effects of photographic evidence on mock juror judgment. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 12, 30-41.